5
The
Second Brain Boom
What kicked
in, about 750,000 years ago?
The first brain
boom started with the advent of the
Homo
spinoff, 2.5 million years back. Over the next 1.5 million years, brain
size doubled. Because body size was also growing (and that alone will
increase brain size), this doubling in size is not as impressive as it
originally looked.
Plotting the skull sizes from all the hominid species against time, it is
apparent that brain size started growing more rapidly about 750,000 years
ago. And not much of this second brain boom can be attributed to a parallel
enlargement in the body itself.
So what’s going on here? No one knows because the data are so sparse. It’s
easy to produce a list of things that had to get started sometime in the
last 7 million years. Indeed, there are a hundred differences between the
great apes and humans and most cannot be pinpointed in time.
Paleoanthropology is like a jigsaw puzzle with lots of pieces missing – and
those pieces that you dig out of the ground are so “fuzzy around the edges”
that they seem to fit in a number of places.
The
australopithecines endured until
about a million years ago, looking more and more heavily built, like the
gorillas. When they died out, it left
Homo
erectus
as the only hominid game in town. Starting at about 800,000 years ago,
Homo antecessor
was found in Spain, but no one knows whether it evolved there or in Africa –
or what it was doing differently. Homo erectus carried on elsewhere.
While toolmaking doesn’t seem to change much at this time, several other
parts of the puzzle do. This is about when the ice age climate rhythms are
modified. The major drivers are well known. The tilt of the earth’s axis
changes from 24.6° down to 22.0° and back over a cycle lasting 41,000
years. The month of the earth’s closest approach to the sun, when we get
about 10 percent more energy, is currently in early January but it will
drift around to July in another 12,000 years or so, depending on what the
other planets are doing (that’s why it varies from a 19,000- to a
26,000-year cycle). And, for similar reasons, the shape of the elliptical
orbit around the sun changes from rounder to more elongated over cycles that
mix a minor 100,000-year and a stronger 400,000-year component. The three
rhythms combine to produce a complex fluctuation.
The global amount of water tied up in ice sheets can be estimated from
sea-floor cores, although this does not give a very complete picture of
changing climate. Up until about 750,000 years ago, the successive meltoffs
of ice were about 41,000 years apart, dominated by the tilt cycle. More
recently, the period between major meltoffs has been closer to 100,000 years
and the amplitude of the cycle has been greater.
Since the astronomical factors are unlikely to have changed strength or
rhythm, and the astronomical 100,000-year component is relatively weak, the
change is probably a matter of an alteration in how the earth resonates with
the driving rhythms. For example, it takes time for ice sheets to become
heavy enough to depress the earth’s crust, and when they melt it takes time
for the surface to spring back up. As for why the rhythm shifted when it
did, no one knows but the most recent reversal of the earth’s magnetic poles
was about 780,000 years ago (and some people think we are getting close to
another reversal now).
The increase in the height of the swings suggests that other things might be
going on, such as more instability of the sort associated with the big
abrupt climate changes. The paleoclimate records aren’t sufficiently good
to see very much in detail except the last ice age, but at least one massive
type of abrupt climate change, the Heinrich events associated with the
collapse of the Hudson Bay ice sheet, can be seen as far back as 1.1 million
years ago.
More demanding
hunting techniques are one
possibility for why brain size starts increasing faster. Perhaps techniques
changed from running down prey to projectile predation. It could be a shift
from side-of-the-barn throws at large targets (say, herds bunched up while
visiting the waterhole) to more accurate throws involving individual prey
animals at greater distances. It is only the accurate throws that make a
lot of demands on the brain, compared to what great apes can do.
All throws require some planning during get set, and so even the inaccurate
throws might instill some preparatory traits that could carry over to
preparing for other ballistic tasks (say, clubbing and hammering). But
accurate throws really have to tune up this neural machinery. If my Law of
Large Numbers analysis in
The
Cerebral Code
proves correct, you have to borrow some inexpert areas of the brain
temporarily to assist the expert areas, much as the amateur audience assists
the expert choir in singing
The
Hallelujah Chorus.
So the get set prelude involves a major amount of reassignment of cerebral
resources, quite unlike most cognitive tasks. Once you start refining
accuracy, however, there is that long growth curve where payoff increases
with each redoubling of distance achieved with accuracy.
Protolanguage is
my other candidate for what might fit
the jigsaw puzzle for this period. While the burst of creativity about
50,000 years ago was originally credited to the onset of language, the last
half-century of research on language origins suggests that language is a
two-step affair – or, rather, a ramp of improvements followed by a big step
up to structured language. Perhaps the ramp was rising about 750,000 years
ago and the step is what comes 50,000 years ago.
Protolanguage is a distinction developed by the linguist Derek Bickerton
from his studies of how unstructured pidgin languages are converted by
children into fully structured creole languages. You can see the same
transition in the stages through which a child develops language. First
come words, then word combinations that have an additional meaning besides
those of the component words. Short sentences do not often require any
notions of structuring to be understood, but once you try to double the
number of words in a sentence, its meaning becomes quite ambiguous without
some structural scaffolding that we call syntax.
Still, you can say quite a lot with two-word sentences, compared to what
other animals accomplish with their one-call-one-meaning vocalizations.
While the child’s passion for naming things might lead us to think that
nouns are the big thing, the emotional expressions of other primates are a
lot closer to verbs.
Most words are a bit abstract, more categories than labels for an individual
or a place. Proper nouns are considerably more difficult, it seems, than
categories. For example, a brain-damaged patient might be able to name the
make and model of cars in a series of photographs but be unable to pick out
a picture of his own car. So the evolution of language abilities might not
be “naming the creatures” so much as a series of prompts for action,
equivalents of “Let’s go” and “Look there.”
Some such foundation is likely how protolanguage got started. Unlike the
onset of syntax (more in a moment), it certainly looks as if protolanguage
progress could be gradual, with no really big steps needed, just a growing
vocabulary and then two words paired for a third meaning. It could have
been going on for several million years, or it might have begun only 160,000
years ago with the advent of anatomically modern
Homo
sapiens.
No one knows. But neocortex is all about forming new associations between
concepts, and neocortical size expands more than fourfold between the great
apes and modern humans. So it would not be surprising if the novel meanings
for word combinations might have promoted, or profited from, a bigger brain.
Body postures communicate mood and intention (dogs communicate dozens), and
arm or face posture sequences provide even more bandwidth for broadcasting
your emotions and intentions. Species-specific vocalizations get a big
addition from culturally defined “words” (whether signed or spoken) whose
learned meaning depends much more on context.
Word combination is just another example of context dependence, but it was
likely an important step. Yet with only protolanguage, you couldn’t say
“Who did what to whom” much faster than you could pantomime it. You’d have
to make a series of short sentences rather than one compact structured
sentence. Pantomime tends to be appealing as an early stage but acting it
out might not have been so common early on. Pretense is involved, and
playing a role (where your actions are to be interpreted as those of someone
else) might not have come along until structured thought arose. Still, you
can do a lot without much pretense – say, pounding on something while
looking at a third party might communicate to your friend what happened in
her absence, just by simple association.
It was originally supposed that coordinating hunts was a big early payoff
for language – until it turned out that chimps had all the basic moves
without using vocalizations. Now it is supposed that much of the everyday
payoff for language has to do with socializing and sexual selection, where
“verbal grooming” and gossip become important players. Again, it looks as
if a gradual improvement ought to work, and that identifying starting times
is likely to be less relevant than finding periods of more rapid progress.
Words are tools in some sense and extend the realm of thought beyond the
here-and-now. There is one class of words that might have been particularly
handy as protolanguage progressed in an era of hunting. These are the
“closed class” words (so called because, unlike nouns and verbs, it is so
hard to invent a new one) that serve to orient you. Some indicate relative
direction
(to, from, through, left, right, up, down)
in the manner of vectors.
Words such as
above,
below, in, on, at,
next to,
and
by
serve to orient you relative to other objects. In the brain, such spatial
tasks tend to involve midbrain areas such as superior colliculus and the
“where” specializations of the upper parts of both parietal lobes. Because
the frontal lobe tends to be involved with planning, perhaps the
closed-class words for relative time (before,
after, while,
and the various indicators of tense) might live up there instead.
While there is an elementary sort of directional “structure” involved here,
it is not open ended in the sense of being expandable (in the manner of
nouns and verbs). The reason that it’s a closed class of words is that it
is about coordinates, and you only need so many words for the four
dimensions of space-time, even when you add relative terms to relate several
objects and their movements (inside, beneath, alongside, atop).
Such are not the type of recursion and nesting that constitutes the big step
up to syntax – and, more generally, structured thought – with its open-ended
nature. Still, the acquisition of these “little words of grammar” would
have made short sentences much more versatile and hominid mental life even
less like that of the great apes. With them, you could begin to order the
world.
All humans do it.
Gossip, schmooze, chitchat, gab, talk, tattle, rap, banter, discuss, debate,
and chew the fat. Why? To exchange information, share knowledge,
criticize, manipulate, encourage, teach, lie, and self-promote.
– Marc D.
Hauser, 2000
Seeing has, in our culture, become synonymous with
understanding. We “look” at a problem. We “see” the point. We adopt a
“viewpoint.” We “focus” on an issue. We “see things in perspective.” The
world “as we see it” (rather than “as we know it” and certainly not “as we
hear it” or “as we feel it”) has become the measure for what is “real” and
“true.”
– Gunther Kress and Theo van Leeuwen,
1996