6
Neanderthals and Our Pre-sapiens
Ancestors
Two-stage toolmaking and what it says about thought
By about
400,000 years ago, brain size is
beginning to overlap with the modern range of brain size – the average
isn’t there yet, but some individuals then had brains just as large as
many people of normal intelligence now have. Though
Homo erectus
carried on in most of Asia, there was a new species called
Homo heidelbergensis
in Africa and Europe, perhaps the common ancestor of both our
sapiens
lineage and that of the Neanderthals.
Some spectacular evidence for advanced projectile predation
is seen about then, suggesting that less advanced forms had been around
for some time before. One spear discovered in a coal mine at
Schöningen, Germany, has a split shaft for hafting, suggestive of
mounting sharp points. Found lying among stone tools and the butchered
remains of ten horses in layers dated to about 400,000 years ago, there
are also three wooden spears several meters long. They were made from
the trunks of spruce trees that were about 30 years old. After removing
the bark, they were sharpened at the base of the trunk, where the wood
is hardest. The thickest and heaviest part of the carved shaft is about
a third of the distance back from the spear tip.
So these three weren’t the crude beginnings of spears, of
the sort useful for thrusting and keeping troublesome scavengers at a
distance. Balanced like modern javelins, they were surely thrown.
The third big
advance in crafting stones also
occurred about this time, staged toolmaking. Like staged food
preparation, it suggests that hominids had learned to think ahead and
prepare an intermediate product.
Producing a sharp edge seems to have been the major
preoccupation of toolmakers, starting 2.6 million years ago. Though
random bashing works (shatter and search), it is wasteful of raw
material and, if you don’t happen to live in the midst of plenty, you
have the motivation to make your hand-carried rock produce more sharp
edges than random bashing produces. And so, early on, directed blows
became common. The idea may not have been to construct a particular
shape of tool, so much as to get more sharp edges per rock.
The second stage, known as the Acheulean, involved both hard
hammering for the basic shapes and soft hammering for the edges. The
handaxe was only the most enigmatic of the Acheulean tools. This was a
gradual working of the stone to achieve a desired form, though not
staging per se.
What developed was a method of staging, controlling the
shape of the struck-off piece so that it had two good edges in a
V-shape. The knapper first shaped a rock to have two sloping platforms
like a tent. Then in the second stage, the knapper would up-end this
“core” and strike in a line almost parallel to the “ridgeline.” The
“flakes” struck off would thus be triangular, with two sharp edges
intersecting at the ridgeline. The successive flakes would get bigger
as they worked down through the core. Called Levallois flakes after the
Paris suburb where they were first discovered, they are also common in
Africa in the same period.
Whole books
are regularly written about
Neanderthals, and no wonder. They represent “the path not taken” (that
is, by the way, an advanced form of metaphor; more later) by our
ancestors, yet a path that led to a species that thrived for a long time
in Europe and the Middle East.
Did they lack our kind of hunting techniques? (They
certainly suffered a lot more nonfatal injuries, and died at earlier
ages, than comparable “paleoindian” populations, so perhaps they
regularly got too close to thrashing animals, or regularly beat up on
one another.) Did they lack our kind of tools? (That case was once
made from the archaeological record, but now it seems they had
comparable tools to Cro-Magnons where they overlapped in their
habitats.) Could they make such tools, or just trade for them? Invent
them, or only copy them?
Did Neanderthals come to a violent end at the hands of our
ancestors? Die from imported Cro-Magnon diseases, the way smallpox
cleared the way for European settlers in the Americas? Or were they
simply outcompeted, slowly declining into disappearance as their hunting
grounds shrank during a bad drought? (That’s what evolutionary theory
suggests ought eventually to happen to one of two species occupying the
same evolutionary niche.) There are no answers to most such questions,
but “all of the above” seems likely at different times and places.
Did
Neanderthals have our kind of language?
A theme of many novels is the potential conflict between Neanderthals
and modern
Homo sapiens
because one had the bleached skin of sunshine-starved Europeans and the
other had the dark skins of those fresh out of sunny Africa.
Cross-group romances and cross-rearing of orphans allow the novelist to
show the contrasts, head to head. But the major ploy of the
paleonovelist (many of whom are well informed about the anthropology) is
to assume that one group had our kind of language and the other didn’t.
That is indeed a key scientific question, but no one has yet
found evidence that is widely persuasive on the issue of whether
Neanderthals had language. Still, the effort so far shows the
candidates and they say something about what had happened in the 1.2
million years between early
Homo erectus
and
Homo heidelbergensis.
Since women and children speak at a higher pitch than adult
men, thanks to their smaller size, the hearing part of our brain needs
to correct for this, so that words from a small vocal tract still are
recognized as the same as the words spoken from a deeper throat. Some
of the vowel sounds that we make are useful as calibration signals and
cause us to treat the other speech sounds in a manner appropriate to the
size of vocal tract that produced them. The upper part of the vocal
tract of Neanderthals is not shaped the same as ours, judging from the
curvature of the base of the skull. The argument about Neanderthal
abilities has been about speech, not language per se, and has been
founded on the observation that the Neanderthal throat would not have
been well suited for the production of the vowels
a,
i,
and
u.
Fine, perhaps they calibrated speech sounds in some other
way. Or just slowed down, the way we do when conversing with someone
with a speech defect or a hearing problem. What we really want to know
is how much Neanderthals and our ancestors talked and, if they could,
whether they had protolanguage or, better yet, syntax.
Well, how about the size of the nerve that controls
movements of the tongue? (You can infer it from the size of the hole in
the base of the skull where the hypoglossal nerve exits.) It’s bigger
in us than in great apes or
Homo erectus.
Neanderthals are like us in this regard. But blood vessels also travel
through the same holes as the nerves, and you cannot control for that,
so we are not entitled to conclude that bigger holes were for bigger
nerves. Worse, nerves are bundles of many nerve fibers and there are
two major ways that they can get bigger: more fibers (the usual
assumption, with its implication of finer control), and fibers whose
diameter is greater because they have more fatty insulation. (More
insulation makes them conduct faster, and also lowers the metabolic
costs.)
The other line of evidence comes from the size of the nerves
controlling breathing. Same results: Neanderthals are enlarged much
like we are, but poor
Homo erectus
back at 1.6 million years ago had to get along with ape-sized nerves.
Same caveats, too. Still, larger nerves do indeed suggest finer control
of the chest muscles, and that is something that you would expect to
improve with the careful modulation of breathing needed for speech – or
for that matter, swimming, sustained running, and blowing at embers to
keep the fire going.
None of this proves that Neanderthals indeed had our
vocalization capabilities. Both lines of nerve size evidence, taken
together, suggest that finer control of tongue and chest movements
developed sometime between 1.6 million years ago and about 400,000 years
ago when we shared a common ancestor with the Neanderthals – and that is
at least consistent with a lot more vocalization.
Staging
emerges as the central feature
that says something about the mental capabilities of the hominids of
this period.
You have to be
able to “see” a standard series of blades within the core, something
like imagining sliced bread and shaping the loaf accordingly.
But, like staged food preparation, it is
a routine sort of staging, passed on to an apprentice. It takes time to
get good at it, just as young chimps take six years to get good at nut
cracking. This sort of staging is not necessarily a new set of stages
each time, the way a short-order cook can juggle an order from a whole
table of people and have this unique combination all finish up at the
same time.
Remember those javelins, however. That’s an advanced sort
of projectile predation, and surely only some of the throws were set
pieces performed in a stereotyped ambush setting. So their brains were
likely busy during “get set,” trying to create a novel set of movement
commands.
The issue is how much of that novel staging carried over to
planning other things. If they could stage both toolmaking and food
preparation, perhaps their life of the mind included other kinds of
agendas as well, with more of an eye to the future. Maybe they added
“ready” to the front end of “get set, go” just as accurate throwing and
hammering prefaced the apelike “go” with a hominid “get set” phase.
To get ready, you have to set the stage with the right
stuff, slowly get everything into position. Getting set requires
orchestrating everything offline. And for ballistic actions, launching
is entirely on automatic because feedback is too slow to modify the
movement. So perhaps the evolutionary ordering is go-set-ready.