The view from the community hearth outside the ceremonial cave?
(Grotte de Font de Gaume, France)
Preface
[History] is concerned not with events but
with processes. Processes are things which do not begin and end but which
turn into one another.
– R. G.
Collingwood, 1939
There is something about a big campfire.
Small cooking hearths are very useful but, beginning about 120,000 years
ago in South Africa, the archaeologists start finding them supplemented
by a bigger hearth. Psychologically, it’s very attractive – a community
bonfire pulls in people from all round the camp.
Back then, did someone tell
origin stories around the campfire? That date, in the middle of the
prior warm period in the ice ages, is an enigmatic date, as you’ll
discover about halfway through my origin story.
Homo sapiens
was around by then – they looked a lot like us, big brains and all – but
behaviorally they weren’t yet
us,
the innovative species known as
Homo sapiens sapiens,
a people not doubly wise so much as far more creative. It wasn’t until
about 50,000 years ago that they were finally doing things that cause us
to say, “They must have thought a lot like we do.” At that point, they
surely appreciated campfire storytelling.
"Can you tell the story of the world in
an evening around the campfire, the way an old-fashioned shaman used to
do?" This was the challenge that the historian David Fromkin took up in
writing his short book, The
Way of the World. It mostly
focused, as historians do, on the time scale of civilizations, going
back perhaps 6,000 years. There are other admirable short histories
which have inspired me, such as Stephen Hawking’s
A Brief History of Time,
on the cosmological time scale of the 13 billion years that began with
the Big Bang.
My origin story starts at 7
million years ago, so as to cover the
time since we emerged from the great
apes. To understand the emergence of mind – and particularly the higher
aspects of consciousness that
so set us apart from the rest of the animal kingdom – we need to
understand what the great apes are capable of. And what they don’t
do. That will help us appreciate what happened in ape-to-human brain
evolution since we last shared a common ancestor with the chimpanzees
and bonobos of Africa.
It is just in the last 1
percent of that up-from-the-apes period that human creativity and
technological capabilities have really blossomed. It’s been called “The
Mind’s Big Bang.” In our usual expansive sense of “mind,” the history
of the mind is surprisingly brief, certainly when compared with the long
increase in brain size and the halting march of toolmaking. What came
before was not, as we usually assume, a series of increasing
approximations to the modern mind. So what set the stage for this
creative explosion?
The modern mind of
Homo sapiens
sapiens
is so startling, when seen against its evolutionary background, that it
is worth the effort to tell the up-from-the-apes story in a space short
enough so that all the intermediate stages will linger together in the
reader’s working memory, reverberating off one another, creating a
living contrast that might help illuminate mind’s future.
There are many ways to write a book like this, depending on the
author’s viewpoint. We all tend to deal with the same set of facts, but
our intellectual backgrounds and interests differ. Most people writing
on the subject were trained in anthropology, linguistics, psychology, or
evolutionary biology.
I tend to look at the problem
from the standpoint of a neurobiologist, always trying to figure out how
nerve cells can analyze the world, make sensible plans for movement, and
manage those interneurons that convert thought into action. This is the
brain mechanic’s time scale of how. I was driven to looking into
the evolutionary setup for why things work the way they presently
do. And, since I try to deal with brain circuitry for language and
creative plans, I was looking for insights from the comparison of human
brains to those of our closest cousins that lack these behaviors. I
tend to be impressed by self-organization, emergent properties of neural
circuitry, and fast tracks in evolution. For better or worse, this book
reflects those issues more than would be found in most books on human
evolution. Read widely.
Like most brain researchers,
I am inconsistent in using the term ‘mind.’ Yes, the brain does it
all. It is something like the software-hardware distinction – but we
are really dealing with the advanced products here, higher intellectual
function, and ‘mind’ is the term that gets across the complexities.
This is not a brief history of the brain.
We
tend to see ourselves as the narrator of a life story, always
situated at a crossroads between past and future, swimming in
speculation. We can construct alternative explanations for how we got
where we are, emphasizing one aspect or another as a path. Looking
ahead, we imagine various trajectories. We refine our guesses, editing
out the nonsense, and achieve a clearer glimpse of our crossroad
choices.
Because our less imaginative
ancestors couldn’t think about the future in much detail, they were
trapped in a here-and-now existence. They could anticipate routine
happenings (like meals), but not in our extended sense of speculation
and worry. No “what if” and “why me?” They were conscious in the sense
of choosing between alternative courses of action, but with their
unstructured type of mental life, you couldn’t narrate a life story or
conceive of dying someday. Without creative intelligence, there’s no
crossroad and no end of the road.
I intend this brief history
of the mind to itself be a vista from a crossroads, looking back at
simpler versions of mental life, taking stock of what we have now, and
then speculating about mind’s future. For we are at a crossroads in
another sense, that of a frontier where the rules are about to change,
where mind shifts gears again.
That’s my brief history
(you’ll have to provide your own campfire). Instead of starting with a
big bang, I lead up to one – and then look beyond, to contemplate mind’s
next advances.
The more we learn about what we are, the
more options we will discern about what to try to become. Americans have
long honored the “self-made man,” but now that we are actually learning
enough to be able to remake ourselves into something new, many flinch. Many
would apparently rather bumble around with their eyes closed, trusting in
tradition, than look around to see what’s about to happen. Yes, it is
unnerving; yes, it can be scary. After all, there are entirely new mistakes
we are now empowered to make for the first time. But it’s the beginning of
a great new adventure for our knowing species. And it’s much more exciting,
as well as safer, if we open our eyes.
– Daniel C. Dennett, Freedom
Evolves, 2003
THE CLOSEST
COUSINS
Orangutan
(Borneo, also Indonesia)
Gorilla
(Central Africa)
Bonobos
(shown here; left bank of the Congo) are more lightly built than the
otherwise similar chimpanzees (not shown; found in East, Central, and
West Africa).
Some Stage-setting
Perspective
If you have trouble with the names, just
remember that they nest inside one another: Animals > mammals > primates >
monkeys > apes > hominids > us.
While animals have been around perhaps 800 million years,
mammals are seen only in the last 200 million years or so. Primates
evolved from the mammals more than 60 million years ago. Living examples of the
early small-brained prosimian forms include tree shrews, lemurs, the slow loris,
and the galagos.
Monkeys evolved 40 million years ago from the prosimians.
Some of the Old World monkeys lost their tails to become apes about 25
million years ago. The ape brain is about twice the size of a monkey brain;
apes also have more versatile shoulder joints. The lesser apes, the gibbon and
the somewhat larger siamang, are examples of the early apes.
The extant great apes are the orangutan (with whom we
shared
a common ancestor about 12 million years ago), the gorilla (about 8 to 10
million years ago) and the chimpanzee and bonobo (with whom we shared a common
ancestor about 6 or 7 million years ago). They all inhabit forests, though
chimps can sometimes be found in the more open woodlands.
Hominids (hominins in new-speak) are all the species
between that last common ancestor and us humans. They are upright in posture,
live in the woodlands between forests and grasslands, and have lost the big
canine teeth of the apes. But brain size doesn’t change much until 2.5 million
years ago with the earliest Homo species, and that’s about when sharp
stone toolmaking starts. By Homo erectus at 1.8 million years ago, they
were eating a lot of meat and were probably inhabiting the grasslands and no
longer nesting in trees.
For this brief history of the mind, I will start about 7 million
years ago when we shared a common ancestor with the chimp and the bonobo (the
misnamed “pygmy chimp” of central Congo), the two great apes with which we have
the most in common. The width of the Congo River has kept bonobos isolated for
the last several million years from the common chimpanzees, which extend from
the East African Rift Valley in Uganda all the way to Senegal in westernmost
Africa. Behaviorally, bonobos and chimps have different styles, each of which
give us some clues as to what that common ancestor (call it
Pan prior)
might have been thinking, just 7 million years back.
|
If you read the book on the web (uncomfortable but
possible), consider buying a book as a gift for a friend.
(We live and learn and pass it on.)Click on a cover for the link to amazon.com.
A Brief History of the
Mind
2004
A Brain for All Seasons
2002
Lingua ex Machina
2000
The Cerebral Code
1996
How Brains Think
1996
Conversations with
Neil's Brain
1994
The River That
Flows Uphill
1986
The Throwing Madonna
1983 |