The big
thinkers in the sciences of human nature have been adamant that
mental life has to be understood at several levels of analysis, not
just the lowest one. The linguist Noam Chomsky, the computational
neuroscientist David Marr, and the ethologist Niko Tinbergen have
independently marked out a set of levels of analysis for
understanding a faculty of the mind. These levels include its
function (what it accomplishes in an ultimate, evolutionary sense);
its real-time operation (how it works proximately, from moment to
moment); how it is implemented in neural tissue; how it develops in
the individual; and how it evolved in the species.
–
Steven Pinker, 2002
13
Imagining the House of Cards
Inventing new levels of organization on-the-fly
Level of
analysis is an unavoidable
concept in biology, where the process of coming into being is so
varied. In his history of biological thought, Ernst Mayr distinguished
between proximal causes (physiological stuff, such as the mechanics of
brain operation) and ultimate causes (the evolutionary setup phase, what
makes the proximate mechanisms what they now are). Our present
distinction of development (what children do) from evolution (what
species do) seems natural but, in Darwin’s day, the terms were
different. Indeed, Darwin didn’t much employ the term “evolution” as,
back then, it simply implied a pattern unfolding, as in a dance or a
coordinated military maneuver (what marching bands now do at halftime.
So evolutionary setup, mechanics, and development are the main levels of
analysis (or explanation) in biology.
A level of organization is a more general concept, seen in
all of the sciences. This kind of level is best defined by certain
functional properties, not anatomy. As an example of four levels,
fleece
is organized into
yarn,
which is woven into
cloth,
which can be arranged into
clothing.
Each of these levels of organization is transiently stable, with
ratchetlike mechanisms that prevent backsliding: fabrics are woven, to
prevent their disorganization into so much yarn; yarn is spun, to keep
it from backsliding into fleece.
Neuroscientists once talked at cross purposes when arguing
about learning – is it an alteration at the level of gene expression,
ion channels, synapses, neurons or circuits? All of the above?
Confusion of levels also occurred in evolutionary science in the wake of
Darwin when geneticists in the 1900s thought that the newfound genes
were an alternative explanation to natural selection. It is common to
initially suppose that complementary causes are, instead, competing
explanations.
A proper organizational level is characterized by “causal
decoupling” from adjacent levels; it’s a “study unto itself.” You can
weave without understanding how to spin yarn (or make clothing).
Indeed, Dmitri Mendeleev figured out the periodic table of the elements
without knowing any of the underlying quantum mechanics or the overlying
structural chemistry. Most of the natural sciences need only several
levels of organization. There are, however, at least a dozen levels of
organization within the neurosciences — all of the way up from genes for
ion channels to the emergent properties of cortical neural circuits.
And, if we invent a metaphor, we tack on a new level. Then there are
those developmental and evolutionary levels of explanation.
The closest approximation to a word, in the animal world, is
an emotional utterance such as the chimpanzee’s “What’s that?” or “Get
away from that!” equivalents. Occasionally they can be interpreted as
nouns (“snake” or “eagle”). We humans can combine several utterances
for an additional meaning, say “That’s big.” This opens up a space of
thousands of new meanings. In addition to such relationships, we can
compare items, say “This is bigger than that.” We can even build a new
level, that of relationships between relationships, when we say “Bigger
is better.” It is this on-the-fly construction of a new level, as when
we find an analogy or use figurative speech, that is what makes human
cognition so open ended, totally unlike anything seen elsewhere in
evolution. Nothing in animal communication is in this class.
It is only a Seattle coffee joke,
but it nicely illustrates different levels of mental organization.
The
ascent to higher levels of consciousness begins when you first
contemplate the toothpaste in the morning, when you can operate only at
the level of single words and well-memorized actions.
Relationships between concepts, like speaking in sentences, may first
require priming, with the morning cup of coffee.
Talking about relations between relationships (better known as analogies
or metaphors), as when we say “Bigger is better,” may require a double
espresso.
Poets,
of course, invent figurative speech and compound it into blends. (“The
path not taken.”) Some seem to require a series of stage-setting
maneuvers involving many superstitious practices, some of which involve
substances even more toxic than coffee.
Mental life
can pyramid a number of levels,
thereby creating structure. We see the pyramiding of levels as babies
encounter the patterns of the world around them. A baby first picks up
the short sound units of speech (phonemes), then the patterns of them
called words, then the patterns within strings of words we call syntax,
then the patterns of minutes-long strings of sentences called narratives
(whereupon she will start expecting a proper ending for her bedtime
story).
By the time she encounters the opening lines of James
Joyce’s
Ulysses,
she will need to imagine several levels at once:
“Stately,
plump Buck Mulligan came from the stairhead, bearing a bowl of lather on
which a mirror and a razor lay crossed. A yellow dressinggown,
ungirdled, was sustained gently behind him by the mild morning air. He
held the bowl aloft and intoned: Introibo ad altare Dei.”
As
always, there is the physical setting (piecing together the top of an
old Martello gun tower overlooking Dublin Bay with a full-of-himself
medical student about to shave).
But the
ceremonial words and deliberate pace prompt you to consider the more
abstract level of metaphor. An ungirdled gown and an offering of
lather? Is this – gasp! – an obscene mockery of the Catholic mass, far
more blasphemous than anything that Salman Rushdie might have implied
about Islam? And Joyce is instead celebrated in Ireland?
So much of our intellectual task, not just in reading Joyce
and Rushdie but in interpreting everyday conversation, is to locate
appropriate levels of meaning between the concreteness of objects and
the various levels of category, relationships, and metaphor. You
usually cannot get the joke without locating the correct level of
organization to which it refers, and it is often the alternative
interpretations at different possible levels that makes it so funny.
“And about how many people work here?” the visitor politely
inquires of the boss.
“About half.”
Our minds can
operate on the unreal (“the
missing chair” or “zero”) but it is tropes
that allow us ways of saying “this is like that.” They tack on imagery
with connotations above and beyond the literal meaning. We employ
spatial metaphors such as “soaring spirits” and “falling GNP.” It is
claimed that much of learning is dominated by analogy (the heart is
like a pump) and metaphor (the heart is a pump). Roland
Barthes declared that “no sooner is a form seen than it must resemble
something: humanity seems doomed to analogy.”
Even
in science, extended metaphors personify by giving human characteristics
to charged particles where those of like charge “hate” one another
though those of opposite charge “love” one another. Metaphor is the
first thing we try when working our way into a complex subject, but in
doing science you eventually try to replace it with something better
(though the metaphor may remain handy for teaching). In
seventeenth-century England, the scientists of the Royal Society sought
“to separate knowledge of nature from the colours of rhetoric, the
devices of the fancy, the delightful deceit of the fables.” They saw
the “trick of metaphors” as distorting reality. Yes, but something is
better than nothing.
To keep
creative constructions from being nonsensical,
two tasks are needed. The first, as mentioned earlier, is to judge new
associations for their internal coherence: do they all hang together in
a reasonable, safe way? (Initially, most associations are surely as
incoherent as our dreams, which provide us with a nightly experience of
people, places, and occasions that simply do not fit together.) Awake,
it’s an off-line search for coherence, for combinations that “hang
together” particularly well. Sometimes this provides an emergent
property: the committee can do something that none of the separate parts
could manage.
Second, to spend more time at the more abstract levels in an
intellectual house of cards, the prior ones usually have to be
sufficiently shored up to prevent backsliding. Poets, in order to
compare two candidate metaphors, have to build a lot of scaffolding.
Finding the right combination can be like adding a capstone to an arch,
which permits the other stones to support themselves – as a committee,
they can defy gravity and dispense with the temporary scaffolding, so
slowly assembled with the aid of the writer’s superstitious rituals and
self-medications.
The
loss of a normal adult ability to locate and hold a level of
organization is what the psychiatrists are testing for when they ask you
the meaning of a proverb like “People who live in glass houses shouldn’t
throw stones.” Without being able to stabilize your house of cards at
an intermediate level, you can’t reason by analogy. A psychotic patient
may be both very concrete and hopelessly abstract, as if flipping from
one extreme to the other because unable to settle at the mezzanine for
any length of time.
Finding the appropriate level at which to address a problem – not too
concretely, not too abstractly – is an important aspect of intelligence
that is probably not seen in the great apes. Searching the wrong level
is a common blunder in all of higher intellectual function – when you
don’t “get it,” it is often because you cannot locate the intended level
of reference where everything falls into place.
We can distinguish “John believes there is a Santa Claus”
from “There is a Santa Claus” (if apes ever manage this, it will
surprise a lot of researchers). We usually do it so well that a
persistent breakdown of this ability raises the question of
schizophrenia. Still, most of us easily confuse concepts that can be
approached from different levels of analysis, and sometimes we get stuck
rationalizing the results – as when a statement of moral principle (say,
equity feminism’s equal opportunity and equal pay) gets confused with
demonstrable-but-irrelevant facts about biology (say, when some gender
feminists get upset about reports of brain differences between males and
females and worry that needed social progress will suffer unless this
heresy is vehemently denied).
One of the reasons
we so seldom paint ourselves into a corner or saw off the limb we are
sitting on is that we have all heard one funny, memorable tale or
another about a chap who did just that. And if we follow the Golden
Rule, or the Ten Commandments, we are enhancing our underlying natural
instincts with prosthetic devices that tend to encourage framing the
situations we confront in one way or another.
–
Daniel Dennett,
Freedom Evolves,
2003
A sense of
ethical behavior has some
foundations in primate social life, where even a monkey has a sense of
what he can get away with and what will cause trouble. Framing and a
“theory of mind” add much more capability.
We use our theory of another’s mind all the time in
conversation when we pitch a sentence in a way that takes account of our
listener’s knowledge. I say “brain cell” if I’m not sure my listener
knows the term “neuron.” When you say “he” instead of John Smith, or
“the” instead of “a,” you are implying that what you mention is
something that your listener should know already, because of shared
knowledge or some antecedent communication.
This everyday practice of estimating what another knows or
believes helps you acquire an ability to put yourself in someone else’s
shoes. I’ve saved the topic for here, late in my brief history of mind,
because I suspect that our kind of ethics probably owes a lot to
simulating novel social situations in your head before acting. And you
often operate at a second or third remove: I may jaywalk when there
aren’t children around, but I will detour to the crosswalk when I might
be serving as a role model. I may avoid saying something to X because I
can estimate that X wouldn’t want Y, nearby, to overhear it because Y
would probably tell Z.
Of course, it cuts both ways: if your competitiveness
exceeds your empathy, you may use your mental model to take advantage of
the other person rather than help him avoid a problem – as when trying
to outthink your chess opponent by projecting what the board might look
like, three moves ahead. Pretense involves treating some things as a
harmless game, when competitiveness and deception within the rules can
override other considerations. But they are exceptions carved out of a
broad area in which you are expected to tread lightly, to minimize the
impact on others of your moves.
Some of this may have been around long enough to hardwire
some intuitions into the brain. We have some emotional responses such
as embarrassment, shame, and guilt that are not shared with the other
great apes. (Nor are the new emotions commonly featured in our dreams
in the manner of, say, anxiety.) As Mark Twain said, man is
“the only
animal that blushes. Or needs to.” And the social setting – as in that
chimp patrol – can transform otherwise peaceful, thoughtful individuals
into irrational, suggestible, and emotional brutes.
The
new emotions suggest, to me, the role of a good reputation in future
social dealings. That they have become instincts or intuitions makes me
suspect that some protostructure and protolanguage might have been
present in social life for a long time, well before the last transition.
We have
achieved an extraordinary ability
to pretend, fantasize, lie, deceive, contrast alternatives, and
simulate. But levels are the real stuff of creativity, so let me give
an appreciation of one of the greatest feats of creativity: the
everyday emergence of new levels of organization.
Here
is an example of two input spaces serving to prompt you to construct a
third hybrid space in your mind. It is from a sailing magazine
reporting on a “race” between two boats whose journeys were actually 140
years apart:
“As we went to
press, Rich Wilson and Bill Biewenga were barely maintaining a 4.5 day
lead over the ghost of the clipper Northern Light, whose record
run from San Francisco to Boston they’re trying to beat. In 1853, the
clipper made the journey in 76 days, 8 hours.”
We
deal easily with such metaphorical constructions, mapping the old
journey onto our trajectory planning for the modern trip to create a
“ghost” lagging behind. Understanding one story by mapping it onto a
more familiar story (that’s what constitutes a parable) shows how we can
operate mentally, once we have the structure for syntax and can use it
again for even more abstract, beyond-the-sentence constructions. We map
actions between the spaces but perhaps substitute new actors. We do
something similar in logical reasoning.
Blended spaces draw
from several source frames that are closer to reality. The resulting
“blend” inherits qualities from each input but often achieves some
unique properties of its own. The
blending process lets you suggest to your listener that there are
connections between elements, even though their properties may be
materially different. Blending says a
lot about our creativity, as in this description by Mark Turner in
The Literary Mind:
Certainly there is considerable evidence that blending is a mainstay of
early childhood thought. A two‑year‑old child who is leading a balloon
around on a string may say, pointing to the balloon, "This is my
imagination dog." When asked how tall it is, she says, "This high,"
holding her hand slightly higher than the top of the balloon. "These,"
she says, pointing at two spots just above the balloon, "are its ears."
This is a complicated blend of attributes shared by a dog on a leash and
a balloon on a string. It is dynamic, temporary, constructed for local
purposes, formed on the basis of image schemas, and extraordinarily
impressive. It is also just what two‑year‑old children do all day long.
True, we relegate it to the realm of fantasy because it is an impossible
blended space, but such spaces seem to be indispensable to thought
generally and to be sites of the construction of meanings that bear on
what we take to be reality.
Levels
and blending do make you realize that understanding the underlying
neural processes has enormous potential for further enhancing our
cognitive processes – that the last transition might someday become only
the penultimate transition.